Peers are one of the key agents of socialization in a child's
life. For children and particularly adolescents, peers often
constitute their main reference group. In other words, children
and adolescents generally compare themselves, what they have,
and how they behave to their age mates. In fact, adolescents
often guide their own behavior and make decisions based on
what their peers say and on their perceptions of what their
peers think. This process of orientation toward peers grows
in importance with age. It peaks in early and middle adolescence,
which is one of the reasons why this period in a youth's life
is often difficult, and also why delinquency rates are higher
during that time. Then, toward the late teens and for others
in young adulthood, age mates recede in importance as the
young person is placed in social situations that cut across
age groups. Furthermore, by that time, individuals' self-concept
is more stable and peer influence becomes less salient as
new adult roles are assumed.
In our society, the peer group constitutes a particularly
potent form of influence on children because it is embedded
in a materialistic culture based on consumerism, immediate
gratification, and mediadriven activities. From a macrosociological
perspective, children and youth are manipulated on many fronts
by larger social forces that are individualistic rather than
familistic and communal. Youths are situated in an acquisitive
nexus of consumer-driven forces. Thus, because peers are an
adolescent's reference group, this reference group is itself
impacted by larger social forces that become symbolic or ever
virtual points of reference for children.
In the past, children's peer groups were less separated from
the adult world and less age graded in terms of values, goals,
and even activities. No virtual or "mediatic" voices
and persons sold them an ever-changing lifestyle that separated
them from the adult world. Children played together and even
worked together but they were more likely to encounter adult
authority figures who would provide them with a structure
that maintained them within the basic realities of their families
and their communities. Adults were then far more effective
agents of collective socialization; functional communities
contributed to integrate children and youth (Coleman and Hoffer,
1987).
As the peer group separated from the adult world and became
more influential, parents' influence diminished. In fact,
unless children are part of an effective community, peer influence
may clash with parental influence several times daily. As
adult influence in the form of teachers, clergy, and older
kin became more distant, parents remained children's main
source of adult influence in their lives. Therefore, in terms
of socialization, parents were isolated, no longer surrounded
by adult support, and, as such, their influence waned. Similarly,
as peers became more important in a child's life, they became
so in parents' lives. Children are now more affected by their
peers and this effect spills over into family dynamics, hence
the importance of studying the effect of children's peers
on parents.
PEER PRESSURE AND SUPPORT
There is a general adult perception that children and youth
engage in unacceptable behavior as a direct result of their
peers "pressuring" them. By this definition, direct
pressure involves an element of coercion, such as when a small
child is told by an older bully to hand over his lunch money
"or else I'll beat the shit out of you." Or when
a domineering teen gives another one a cigarette and orders
him to smoke it. Or when, at a party, a group of boys forces
another to engage in sexual activities. Of course, all of
these events do occur occasionally, particularly in deviant
areas or peer groups, and when children are poorly supervised.
Researchers, however, define these instances as forms of bullying
rather than peer pressure-and we disuss bullying later on.
In terms of peer pressure, these occurrences -present the
exception rather than the norm.
Peer pressure is more subtle than bullying. It resides in
the fact tha post of a child's friends and contemporaries
engage or are thought to ngage in an activity and not in the
fact that they will force him or he to do it as well. Children
whose friends are studious receive an indi ect message that
perhaps they should also do their homework, espe cially if
they value their peers' opinion. This is what we will call
a studious" peer environment. Thus, children feel peer
pressure ever hough no one has asked them to do anything.
In terms of interactionism, friends are an important reference
group that contribute not only to how children see themselves
but how they choose to -have. But some peer pressure also
arises out of a child's erroneous perception that "everyone
else has one or is doing it," while, in real ty, this
is not the case. For instance, adolescents often brag about
fabulous weekends or parties when they return to school on
Mondays /et they were simply at home with their parents. But
these "stories' lead others who had also been home to
think of themselves a deprived. In turn, they feel pressured
to "get a life" or "live my life."
A teenager whose friends all smoke cigarettes or marijuana
may not be asked to share in their activities, but will feel
left out if he or she does not. If things are not going too
well at home and the child has weak self-esteem, he or she
may feel compelled to join in for fear tha friends think he
or she is "uncool" or a "loser." Slowly,
a message forms in the child's mind and this is where the
pressure exists: it i psychological, although its source is
environmental. Peer pressure resides in the cultural climate
of the peer group and permeates activi ies as well as ways
of thinking. At times, a particularly popular am influential
peer engages in certain activities and has a strong impac
on slightly younger peers, as seen in the following student's
autobiography.
This twosome of Pete and I became a threesome when we me
Alan who was a saxophone player in the band. The three of
u got along very well.... It wasn't until the end of grade
eight that we started to feel Alan's influence over us when
he introduce us to cigarettes. This influence was brought
about by the fact that Alan had an older brother who was
involved in a group of friends which was made up of what
I considered "tough" people. Alan had tried smoking
and one day he brought a cigarette with him to school. The
three of us went out back at recess and Alan sparked it
up. I remember the taste in my mouth after that first puff;
it was awful but it was exciting at the same time.... Our
parents didn't suspect a thing as we would do everything
we could to mask the smell. One day as we sat and smoked
together behind the school, Alan told us of how his brother
had tried smoking pot. The three of us were very interested
and arranged for Alan's brother to get us some to try. A
few weeks later it happened. It was Saturday and the three
of us went to the forest by Pete's house. Alan pulled out
a bag of what looked like a few hand-rolled cigarettes.
We smoked them and nothing happened. We were disappointed
but Alan assured us that one was not supposed to get high
the first time. While making the transition from cigarettes
to pot, my home life, appearance, and extracurricular activities
changed. No longer did I communicate with my family, do
homework, or play sports. Instead I started to listen to
heavy rock music, hang out at an arcade, grow my hair long,
and wear ragged clothes.... I had perfected the art of deception,
which kept my family in the dark the whole time. I found
this quite amusing and keeping my parents ignorant was an
exciting game.
The above quote illustrates how negative peer influence may
work, particularly within a small friendship group, and leads
from one slightly deviant activity to a more serious break
with acceptable behavior. The quote also quite vividly describes
how these peer activities actually affect the quality of the
youth's interaction with his family and how they affect his
parents. The direction of causality flows from peer group
to adolescent to parents. This quote also illustrates how
deviant activities shared with friends can distance an adolescent
from prosocial activities such as schoolwork and team sports.
In turn, this distancing will have a further negative impact
on parents who are concerned about their child's sudden loss
of interest in developing his human capital.
Peers who are friends and, even more so, close friends,
naturally influence or can influence one another more forcefully.
In fact, a child who is surrounded by a negative peer group
may well be able 1 resist this influence if he or she happens
to have one or two close friends who have similar expectations
in life, particularly when these two friends have supportive
parents. This combined little group c friends and adults then
has a degree of closure from the rest of the world and protects
its children. But the characteristics of a neighborhood and
its schools largely determine the effect of peer support on
children.
For instance, Dubow, Edwards, and Ippolito (1997) found
that children in inner-city neighborhoods who have recently
suffered stress ful situations exhibit higher levels of antisocial
behavior when the report receiving peer support. In contrast,
family support has a positive effect and is related to reduced
antisocial behaviors. These researchers point out that "the
choice of peers may depend, in great part, on the characteristics
of the neighborhoods in which the children reside" (Dubow,
Edwards, and Ippolito, 1997:141). In other words, areas with
many social problems may have fewer prosoch peers available
to a child so that in times of stress, he or she receive "support"
from peers who may be antisocial. This factor increases th
child's vulnerability both to additional stressors and to
misbehaving. In contrast, in a low-risk neighborhood, Gonzales
and colleague (1996) found that peer support predicted higher
grade point average among African-American children. Therefore,
where a family lives has a great deal of impact on its children's
access to a potentiall prosocial, supportive peer network.
Having high-achieving peers can influence children's and
adolecents' enjoyment of school as well as contribute to raising
their own test scores and expectations (Mounts and Steinberg,
1995). Peer seem to be a relatively more potent source of
influence than parent in terms of school performance among
African- and Asian-America youngsters than among white adolescents
(Steinberg et al., 1995; This stems in part from racial segregation,
if not necessarily always a the residential level, at least
in schools. Asian Americans have "no choice but to belong
to a peer group that encourages and rewards aca demic excellence-for
better or for worse, it is extremely difficult for an Asian
youngster to become a jock,' or a `druggie,' or a `preppie'
(Steinberg et al., 1995:449). The dilemma faced by African-Ameri
can students is more or less the reverse; for them, belonging
to school crowd of "nerds" is difficult. In fact,
Steinberg and colleagues were often told by successful black
students that peer support for academic achievement was so
limited that they often opted to affiliate primarily with
students from other ethnic groups. Black students are far
more likely than others "to be caught in a bind between
performing well in school and being popular with their peers"
(Steinberg et al., 1995:449).
The neighborhood approach to peer support and influence
is more sociological and has the advantage of avoiding the
psychological reductionism whereby peers and children are
described strictly through their personality characteristics
or those of their parents. Indeed, it will not surprise the
reader to learn that a substantial proportion of the research
literature on children's affiliation with peers focuses on
parents and how they treat their children. Basically, the
line of reasoning of this research is as follows: Children
who are not popular with their school crowd or are rejected
by their peers have certain social incompetencies that originate
in their relationship with their parents or the ways in which
they were treated at home. Children who experience difficulties
at home are less likely to be popular at school. And, indeed,
the research finds modest correlations supporting this line
of reasoning.
But the size of these correlations indicates that other
aspects in a child's personality and environment account far
more for popularity with peers than do parents. One can think
here of a child's partly genetic predisposition to shyness,
extroversion, or leadership ability (Stoneman et al., 1999).
One can also think in terms of a child's size, material possessions,
race, and quality of peer group. For instance, an agreeable
and prosocial child who is thrust into a classroom of children
who are aggressive and have an "attitude" is quite
unlikely to be well received. The child may actually become
a victim, although there is obviously nothing wrong with this
child or with his or her parents. |