HOW IS ADOPTION SOCIALLY DEFINED?
In societies of European origins such as ours where family
is equated with biology and kinship with blood ties, a great
deal of ambivalence exists concerning adoption (Wegar, 1992).
In contrast, in other societies, adoption is more widespread
and the adoption of kin may be routine. In North America,
a woman's fertility is still considered an important mark
of self-esteem and social recognition despite a general liberalization
of gender role norms (Letherby, 1994). Biological motherhood
is often considered superior to adoptive motherhood, even
by some feminists such as Phylis Chesler (1989). This new
alliance of biology with feminism presents an anomalous situation
at the ideological level because feminist theories generally
reject gender roles based on biological constraints (Rothman,
1989). Who has not heard the well-known feminist slogan "biology
is not destiny"? Yet infertility and adoption are stigmatized
even from these same well-meaning quarters (Bartholet, 1993).
Other feminists, however, point out that "we can recognize
and appreciate the genetic tie without making it the determining
connection" (Rothman, 1989:39). Still other feminists
and scholars such as Arlene Skolnick (1998) critique this
"new biologicm" as a cultural phenomenon with policy
implications.
It is probably because of this ambivalence and even duality
between what is natural versus different or natural versus
biological that adoption has captured the public interest.
Wegar (1997:97) reveals that, in 1993, adoption was featured
113 times in nationwide radio and television programs in comparison
to 186 times for abortion, 95 times for divorce, and 31 times
for birth control. Furthermore, adoption is often portrayed
in television soap operas. This large number of mentions and
portrayals reflects a preoccupation rather than a reality
because adoption by nonkin is far less frequent in our society
than either divorce, abortion, or particularly birth control.
Adoption obviously has dramatic and mediatic value, in part
because it is socially constructed in an ambivalent way. In
another society with a different social construction of family
life and ties, adoption would go unnoticed.
Adoption is a topic that has been newsworthy for several
decades, but each decade puts a different spin on the matter.
The focus in the 1950s was more practical and centered on
advice given to prospective adoptive parents. The realities
of the baby shortage as well as international and transracial
adoption did not surface until the 1960s. By 1974, sealed
adoption records had become an issue and by the early 1980s
this focus was accompanied by one on the search for biological
parents and subsequent reunions. Birth parents became more
visible socially and more acceptable (Wegar, 1997). They were
also given more rights by the courts. In the 1990s, newscasts
reported on several heartbreaking cases of small children
taken away from the only family they had ever known, crying
and totally devastated emotionally, to be given to a natural
parent who had reappeared in their lives and sued for custody.
Therefore, an evolution has occurred in the cultural description
of issues surrounding adoption and in the consequences for
the children involved. This evolution in itself well illustrates
that ideas about adoption change and are cultural rather than
"natural." At any rate, even something natural,
such as mothering, is redefined or reconstituted as we go
along. The more recent development of reunions of adopted
children with their birth mothers is a case in point. It has
seized the public's interest and has been constructed within
the language of other values that are currently prevailing
in our culture. First, the language used reaffirms the merits
of biological over social parenting (Rothman, 1989; Skolnick,
1998). It equates biology or genes with nurturance and, as
such, accords with the new scientific trends in molecular
genetics. This is referred to as "the DNA mystique"
by Nelkin and Lindee (1995).
Second, search and reunion have come to be equated with
the triumph of nature over the oppression of the adoption
system. Searchmovement activists have depicted the psychological
need to search as a universal one, while there are indications
to the contrary (Wegar, 1997): in other words, not every adopted
child feels a need to search or will remain "incomplete"
without a reunion. What "incomplete" means is also
a social construction but it is an effective imagery because
it currently has high value in our "psychobabble."
Furthermore, this presumed universal "need" to locate
one's roots is couched within other very modern psychological
and individualistic themes such as the need to "find
oneself," for "self-fulfillment," "freedom,"
"choice," and the presumed personal problems and
"repression" of those who do not search. As Bartholet
(1993) points out, the search movement has inadvertently contributed
to the further stigmatization of adoption and particularly
adoptive parents, as well as of adopted children who are not
interested in finding their birth parents.
THE PROFESSIONAL REPRESENTATION OF ADOPTED
CHILDREN
The literature on adoption is divided between those researchers
who have not found any significant difference in adjustment
levels and outcomes between adopted and nonadopted persons
and those who do (for a review, see Sharma, McGue, and Benson,
1998). A few studies even find that adopted children show
certain ego strengths and resil-ience (Benson, Sharma, and
Roehlkepartain, 1994). But, overall, adopt, children and their
parents do not show any appreciable deficit that consistent
and would warrant concern (Bartholet, 1993). Nevertheless
superficial reviews of the research reported in popular outlets
tend highlight the negative rather that the positive, a focus
which is unlike to make adoptive parents feel secure in their
role and adopted children secure in their sense of belongingness.
The deficits and strengths that are detected in the studies
depend largely on the methodology used; that is, the type
of adopted children included in the sample (for example, their
age at adoption), the compa son group, and the outcomes measured.
As a result of this research ambi alence, professionals who
come into contact with troubled adopt children or adolescents
may be too hasty to attribute the problems adoption (Miall,
1996). These youngsters may not be treated for the re problems
that affect them: instead, they may be queried about "how
you feel about being adopted." Such a line of inquiry
simply reinforces the adopted child's thinking that something
is wrong with being adopte This will not solve whatever other
problems they might have.
More adopted than nonadopted children, including adoptions
older children, are found among the psychologists' and psychiatrist
clientele (Brodzinsky, 1993). The explanations that have been
offer are twofold. First, some adopted children may have a
more difficult time growing up because of identity problems
or because their parents may not have properly bonded with
them. Here, "bonding theorie constitute a great disservice
to adoptive parents because such theories" often posit
the necessity of bearing a child as well as touching an infa
immediately after birth so that attachment can take place.
If v extended this line of reasoning, one could well say that
husbands ar wives cannot bond because they were not together
at birth!
The second explanation posits that there may be no real
develol mental difference between adopted and nonadopted children.
Rathe what may actually occur is that adoptive parents are
of higher socio economic status and are more familiar with
mental health service Consequently, they may be more inclined
to consult professionals; soon as their children evidence
some problems (Warren, 1992). The may also consult professionals
quickly because they have heard th adopted children "have
more problems." Biological parents, in con trast, may
seek help only after more serious problems have developed.
Hence, comparatively fewer nonadopted children would appear
in the psychologists' statistics.
A third explanation has not been highlighted: it is cultural
and stems from the negative social construction of adoption.
Although adopted children may feel as loved and as accepted
by their parents as nonadopted children, their peers and even
other adults often openly express doubts to them on this topic.
A respondent in March's (1995:656) study said that outside
the family people "never believe that your adoptive
parents love you like their parents love them. Because you
aren't biological." One of my students recalls returning
home one day quite distressed around age ten and asking
her mother, "is it true that you can't love me as much
because I am adopted?" The mother was shocked and found
it difficult to accept the cruelty or incomprehension of
other children. A twelve-year-old girl received this reaction
from a classmate; "Oh, no, you're adopted! You, poor,
poor thing. You don't have real parents." The classmate
shrieked while covering her mouth with her hands, to the
bewilderment of the adopted girl who then told her mother,
"You can imagine that this did not make me feel very
good." Another girl has been asked about her "fake"
mother.
And, of course, parents feel very sorry for their children
and have to explain that adoption is not common enough for
people to be used to it and that they do not understand it,
and so on. Nevertheless, parental explanations and reassurances
may not be sufficient to offset the damage created by peers'
reactions. After all, as we have seen in Chapter 7, peers
are an extremely important reference group in the formation
of a child's self-definition. Thus, this stigmatization may
constitute a heavy mental burden for some adoptees and may
perhaps explain all of the problems that adoption causes to
children's adjustment in life. Instead, if children received
the message that adoption is either neutral or natural or
good, they would experience fewer feelings of ambivalence.
Other parents are saddened when their own parents prefer
grandchildren who are of their "own blood." Or parents
become upset when a teacher says, "Of course, your child
is difficult, but you never know what you get when you adopt
them." Or another points out, "Well, I understand,
you can't quite love them as much as if they were your own."
Then there is the case of a new acquaintance who insists upon
asking, "What about children of your own?" In other
words, the social response constitutes a form of discrimination,
and it is somewhat surprising that researchers have failed
to grasp this phenomenon and its effects on parents and children.
The view on adoptive parents is equally mixed (Groze, 1996).
Some writers have suggested that adoptive parents may be less
confident, more anxious, and harbor feelings of stigmatization
because of their infertility, while other researchers have
not found any support for this "at-risk" perspective
(for a review, see Borders, Black, and Pasley, 1998). Research
questions are unfortunately framed within a pathological or
deficit view of adoption. Such questions assume a social construction
of adoption that creates problems rather than prevents them.
It may perhaps not be surprising that the results are what
they are: the social constructions create the very problems
attributed to the adoption itself. |