Displaced Aggression

Chapter 12 of Moghaddam's Great Ideas in Psychology

 

For audio version (12 minutes), click here.

 

Moghaddam's discussion of displaced aggression is very clear, so I will not add much; however, the comments I do have I will add here rather than in the lecture hour so that we have sufficient time for discussion of the Cosgrove and Flynn study.

 

While there are many other viewpoints, Moghaddam emphasizes the Freudian basis of the displaced aggression concept. Recall that Freud posited aggression as instinctual; that is, he viewed it as built-in to humans. Freud said that some repression (though not total repression) of this instinctual aggression (and of instinctual sexuality) is necessary for the survival of society. In Chapter 4, Moghaddam described repression as a defense mechanism, that is, as a protection against the anxiety associated with unconscious conflicts involving aggression and sexuality. In the current chapter, Moghaddam presents displaced aggression as an example of another defense mechanism and introduces a few others - projection and rationalization. I also mentioned a few defense mechanisms in my lecture on January 15, and a complete list with descriptions can be found by entering "defense mechanisms" in the on-line Oxford Dictionary of Psychology (always available on the left sidebar of the website).

 

Moghaddam also introduces the Freudian concepts of "libido" and "transference."  Freud used libido as the general name for the emotional energy associated with instinctual love in all its forms. However, Freud believed that relations are never fully loving - always also involving hostile instincts - and that in order to meet society's demands and avoid anxiety, humans employ defense mechanisms to repress the negative aspect.  The general meaning of transference is displacement of an emotion or attitude away from its obvious natural object and towards someone else. The most common use of this concept involves relationships in the psychotherapy context - Freud suggested that clients often come to experience the same conflicts and respond emotionally towards their therapists in the same ways that they do with respect to significant others in their lives. Therapists believe this transference is very valuable because an accomplished therapist can work with a client to recognize and change these emotional responses as they appear in therapy, thereby assisting the client to understand them and generalize the changes to daily life.

 

Moghaddam chooses to place his emphasis on the intergroup rather than the interpersonal aspects of displaced aggression. He introduces the important distinction of in-groups and out-groups, and he takes a brief detour to describe two ways in which in-groups are likely to form and cohere. One idea is that groups form as a result of each individual's attraction to similar others, but it is important to attend to Moghaddam's point that the dimensions along which individuals assess similarity is determined by culture - dimensions that are important in one culture may be totally irrelevant in another (consider the relative importance of the Catholic/Protestant distinction in Belfast and in Toronto, for example). He also emphasizes that groups may form under conditions of mutual threat, and that even competing groups may come together by way of the perception that they share a common threat (consider his example of diverse American groups in the immediate aftermath of 9/11).

 

The research of Muzafer Sherif is mentioned as important in the study of intergroup relations, but Moghaddam doesn't describe this work. Perhaps the best-known Sherif study is the Robbers Cave Experiment in which two groups of 12-year-old boys were manipulated in a summer camp setting to compete aggressively with each other and then to resolve their differences through seeking solutions to shared threatening problems. You can read a description of the study by following this Robbers Cave link or by consulting Sherif's own report available from York's Psychclassics website.

 

Moghaddam also mentions Dollard and Miller's frustration-aggression hypothesis in his chapter. Dollard and Miller attempted to formalize many Freudian concepts in more behaviourist learning terms, that is, in a manner that would make them testable. The strongest form of their hypothesis was that aggression is always the result of blocked goals that lead to frustration and then to aggression. A great deal of research was done on this idea, and, as Moghaddam summarizes, researchers quickly saw that there are many possible responses to frustration other than aggression (regression, depression, or lethargy, for example) and that there are other sources for aggression than just frustration (fear-induced aggression, territorial aggression, or aggression instrumental to satisfying desires, for example). For a brief overview of the full range of theories that psychologists have considered with respect to the origins of aggression, see the BBC's H2G2 page, Theories of Aggression (my audio version refers to an article that is no longer available online).

 

Many contemporary textbook authors are much more dismissive than Moghaddam of the displaced aggression idea, and this is likely tied up with the numerous problems found in research on the frustration-aggression hypothesis. However, a recent article (cited in Moghaddam's references) by Amy Marcus-Newhall et al. claims that "displaced aggression is alive and well". Follow the link for the abstract and article, but their conclusion from a review of the numerous laboratory studies (and only laboratory studies) of the phenomena is that displaced aggression does occur and is more likely to be directed at someone perceived as similar to oneself than at someone perceived as dissimilar.

 

Aggression is another of those psychological phenomena that engenders debate over the role of nature and nurture. Freud's theory assumes that aggression is a natural phenomena, and that though it can be repressed, this repression will itself have psychological consequences. Psychoanalysts frequently speak of finding ways to 'sublimate' the natural aggression, that is, to direct it into more socially constructive forms. Social learning theorists, on the other hand, have concentrated on understanding the ways in which aggression is a learned response to social situations. Albert Bandura studied the ways in which children learn aggression through imitation of models they observe in their environment (including learning from the model of parents who spank them for their aggressive behaviour toward playmates). Leonard Berkowitz, mentioned by Moghaddam in relation to the frustration-aggression hypothesis, argued that the typical response to frustration is actually anger rather than aggression and that people learn from their environment what is and what is not appropriate behaviour when one is angry. Developmental Origins of Aggression, a recent book edited by McGill psychologist Richard Tremblay, presents several accounts that blend biological and social accounts of the development of aggression. For a brief review of this book that also summarizes the material presented, click on the linked book title. Finally, Thomas Denson has suggested that displaced aggression can be treated as a personality trait (which doesn't settle whether its origins are nature or nurture). He argues that some people are more inclined than others to indulge in this form of aggression. He has developed a questionnaire, the DAQ, designed to measure the tendency towards displaced aggression. The DAQ and a journal article describing it and the theory behind it are available on Denson's website.

 

ron sheese

March 1, 2008

Last revised February 15, 2010